HUMAN GROWTH
AND DEVELOPMENT
NATURE Vs NURTURE
Individual differences occurring under
the same heredity may be attributed to the operation of different environmental
factors. Similarly, when the environments are sufficiently alike,
dissimilarities of behaviour indicate differing heredity.
A significant question in developmental
psychology is the relationship between innateness (Nature or heredity) and
environmental influence (Nurture) in regard to any particular aspect of
development. This is often referred to as "nature versus nurture" or nativism
versus empiricism.
A nativist account of development would
argue that the processes in question are innate, that is, they are specified by
the organism's genes. An empiricist perspective would argue that those processes
are acquired in interaction with the environment.
Today developmental psychologists
rarely take such extreme positions with regard to most aspects of development;
rather they investigate, among many other things, the relationship between
innate and environmental influences.
Nature and Nurture Defined
Nature refers
to heredity: the genetic
makeup or "genotypes" (i.e., information encoded in DNA) an
individual carries from the time of conception to the time of death. Heredity
may range from genetic predispositions that are specific to each individual and
that therefore potentially explain differences in individual characteristics
(e.g., temperament), to those supposedly specific to certain groups and that
therefore account for group differences in related characteristics (e.g.,
gender and height), and to those that are theorized to be shared by all humans
and are generally thought to set humans apart from other species (e.g., the
language acquisition device in humans).
The
notion of nature, therefore, refers to the biologically prescribed tendencies
and capabilities individuals possess, which may unfold themselves throughout
the course of life.
Nurture, by
contrast, refers to various external or environmental factors to which an individual is exposed from conception
to death. These environmental factors involve several dimensions. For example,
they include both physical environments (e.g., secondhand smoking and prenatal
nutrition) and social environments (e.g., the media and peer pressure). Also, environmental
factors vary in their immediacy to the individual; they involve multiple layers
of forces, ranging from most immediate (e.g., families, friends, and
neighborhoods) to larger contexts (e.g., school systems and local governments)
to macro factors (e.g., international politics and global warming). To
complicate matters even further, the factors in each of these layers influence
and are influenced by elements within and outside of these layers. For example,
the kind of peers a child is exposed to may depend on his or her parents' view
of what ideal playmates are like, the local government's housing policies, and
the history of race relations.
What Is the Controversy?
Despite
its nomenclature, the nature-nurture controversy in its current state is less
dichotomous than commonly believed. In other words, the term
"nature-nurture controversy" suggests a polarization of nature and
nurture; continuity and interaction, however, more aptly describe the central
processes involved in this controversy. Therefore, it is not about whether
either heredity or environment is solely responsible for observed outcomes.
Rather, it is more about the extent to which these factors influence human
development and the ways in which various factors influence each other.
For
example, following the fifteen-person massacre committed by two boys at Columbine High School
in Colorado
in April 1999, the media were flooded with people offering their
interpretations of what drove these high school students to commit this heinous
and violent act. Some were quick to attribute the boys' actions to such
environmental factors as inadequate parenting practices in their families and
the violence prevalent and even glorified in the American media. Others, by
contrast, were convinced that these boys were mentally ill as defined in the
American Psychiatric Association's Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders and that their ability to make responsible judgments had been
impaired, perhaps due to a chemical imbalance to which they were genetically
predisposed. Which argument is "correct," according to most
researchers? Probably neither. Most theorists agree that both nature and
nurture are intertwined and influence most aspects of human emotion, behavior,
and cognition in some ways. Given the prevailing views in current psychology,
most researchers would agree that the violent acts committed by these boys
probably stemmed from an unfortunate interaction among various hereditary and
environmental factors. Researchers, however, may disagree on (1) the extent to
which heredity and environment each influences particular developmental
outcomes and (2) the way in which a mixture of hereditary and environmental
factors relate to each other. In other words, the controversy involves the
extent of contribution as well as the nature of interaction among a variety of
genetic and environmental forces. How do researchers address these issues?
Exploring Heredity and Environment: Research
Methods
Since
as early as the 1930s, researchers have attempted to estimate the contribution
of hereditary and environmental factors to various aspects of human cognition,
by comparing pairs of individuals varying in genetic relatedness. These studies
are often called kinship studies, and twin studies and adoption studies represent
two of the most common types of such studies. They have been extensively
conducted to estimate the heritability of a wide variety of human
characteristics.
Twin Studies
In
traditional twin studies, monozygotic (identical) twins and dizygotic (fraternal)
twins are compared in terms of their emotional, behavioral, and cognitive
similarities. In the process of cell divisions upon formation of a zygote,
sometimes the resulting cells fully multiply and produce two identical babies;
they are called monozygotic twins, since they come from a single zygote and are
genetic "carbon copies." In other words, any genetic information
concerning physical and psychological predispositions should be exactly the
same for these twins.
By
contrast, dizygotic twins develop from two separate zygotes, as a result of two
eggs being fertilized by two sperms independently. Consequently, the genetic
profiles of the resultant babies are similar only to the extent that they share
the same set of biological parents. By comparing the correlations of a
particular dimension, such as intelligence test scores, between identical twins
and those between fraternal twins, researchers can theoretically compute the
relative influences of nature and nurture on the dimension.
For
example, Sandra Scarr reported an interesting finding in the book Intelligence,
Heredity, and Environment. She found a correlation for IQ test scores of .86
for identical twins and .55 for fraternal twins, indicating that identical
twins' scores are more like one another than those of fraternal twins. Some
influence of heredity, therefore, is evident. If IQ scores were 100 percent
genetically determined, however, the correlation for identical twins would have
been 1.00. In this example, therefore, heredity appears to play an important,
but not definitive, role in explaining the determinants of what is measured
through IQ tests.
In
addition to these heritability estimates, researchers also study concordance
rates: the rates at which both twins develop the same, specific characteristics.
The absence or presence of a particular mental illness would be a good example.
If both twins had clinical depression in all pairs examined in a study, then
the concordance rate would be 100 percent for this sample. On the other hand,
if all twins in a study had one individual with clinical depression and another
with no depression, then the concordance rate is 0 percent. Reportedly,
concordance rate for clinical depression is reportedly about 70 percent for
identical twins and about 25 percent for fraternal twins. This appears to
demonstrate a sizable genetic contribution involved in the development of
depression.
Despite
scholars' consensus that genetic contributions are not to be ignored, these
correlational data are often believed to be exaggerated. Identical twins are
genetically predisposed to a great deal of similarities, and, through a process
known as reactive correlation, people around them tend to treat them similarly,
which may help lead the twins to be similar beyond what their genetic profiles
may warrant. The correlation of .86 between the IQ scores of identical twins,
for example, may be contaminated with this reactive correlation. Identical
twins encounter environmental experiences that are extremely similar to each
other's, as the environment tends to react similarly to those who are
genetically similar. As a result, for instance, adults and peers may treat
identical twins similarly, and teachers may also develop similar expectations
about these twins in terms of their emotional, behavioral, and cognitive
functions. This similarity in environmental influences and expectations,
therefore, may cause heritability estimates and concordance rates to be
exaggerated.
Furthermore,
the process of active correlation (or niche-picking) suggests the possibility
that children's genetic predispositions cause them to seek particular
environments, causing the differences in hereditary predispositions to be
enhanced by the subsequent environmental exposure. If a child has the genetic
predisposition to enjoy cognitive challenges, for example, that may prompt the
child to seek situations, friends, and activities that suit this particular
predisposition—provided that such choices are offered to the child. This child,
therefore, may start out with a small genetically prompted inclination to want
to use his or her "brains," but such a tendency would subsequently be
magnified through environmental influences.
Given
the varying degrees of genetic similarities between identical and fraternal
twins, these sources of confusion may theoretically become more consequential
when twins grow up in the same family. This is because twins reared in the same
family are typically subject to the same resources, parenting philosophy,
living environments, and so on. Their genetic predispositions, therefore, are
most likely promoted—or inhibited—in similar ways. For example, if a pair of
twins share the hereditary predispositions for musicality and their
upper-middle-class parents own a piano and are interested in fostering musicality
in these children, their musical potential will perhaps be cultivated in very
similar ways. Specifically, their parents will probably get the same or similar
piano teacher(s) for them, and they will probably be encouraged to practice
equally. Therefore, the genetic similarities between the twins are magnified by
virtue of them growing up in the same household. How does one address these
concerns? Adoption studies provide some answers.
Adoption Studies
Compared
to traditional twin studies, adoption studies are theorized to offer better
alternatives for separating hereditary influences from genetic ones. There are
typically two variations in adoption studies: ones involving comparisons of
identical twins reared apart and ones comparing the degree of similarity
between adopted children and their biological and adoptive parents. Identical
twins reared apart share genetic patterns with each other, yet they do not
share the same environmental experiences. Adopted children, by contrast,
typically share with the rest of the adoptive family similar environmental
experiences but do not share any genes with them. The advantage of adoption
studies is that researchers can reasonably estimate the heritability by
comparing the heritability estimates and concordance rates of pairs of
individuals varying in genetic relatedness and in environmental distance. A
typical adoption study may involve, for instance, comparing the concordance
rates for the following two pairs: a child and her biological parent (shared
genes but not environments) versus the same child and her adoptive parents
(shared environments but not genes). Though the estimates of hereditary
influences are generally lower in adoption studies than in twin studies,
adoption studies provide results that are largely consistent with twin studies.
In a 1983 study, Sandra Scarr and Richard Weinberg found that the IQ scores of
adopted children showed higher correlations with the IQ scores of their
biological parents than with those of their adopted parents. Similarly, John
Loehlin, Lee Willlerman, and Joseph Horn demonstrated through a 1988 study that
in the area of clinical depression, adopted children tended to have much higher
concordance rates with their biological relatives than with their adoptive
relatives.
Still,
many scholars argue that heritability may be overestimated in these studies.
First, the reactive and active correlations discussed earlier would occur, to a
degree, even if the twins were reared separately, as the twins share all of the
hereditary predispositions. Second, one must also examine the possibility that
parents may systematically treat their adoptive children differently than they
do their biological children, which may explain the less-than-expected
resemblance between children and their adoptive parents. Given that
biologically related individuals tend to share greater hereditary similarities,
it is fair to state that heritability estimates may be thrown off by
environmental effects induced by particular genetic predispositions.
Beyond Heritability
As illustrated so far, most psychology researchers
are in agreement that heredity and environment both play significant roles in
the development of various human traits. Researchers may disagree, however, on
the extent to which heredity and environment contribute to the development of a
particular dimension, and on how various factors may affect each other to
create a certain human characteristic. Neither heritability estimates nor
concordance rates provide useful information on the latter type of
disagreement: how various hereditary and environmental factors interact with
each other to result in a particular characteristic. Mental health, education,
and applied psychology researchers are especially concerned about optimizing
the developmental outcomes among people from all backgrounds. To this end,
knowing that there is a .86 heritability estimate for IQ scores among identical
twins, for example, is not particularly helpful in terms of establishing ways
of maximizing the life choices and opportunities for individuals. In attaining
such goals, it is crucial to understand how various factors relate to each
other. Naturally, in order to do so, one must first identify which factors are
involved in the development of a given trait. Unfortunately, researchers have
had very limited success in identifying specific genetic patterns that
influence particular psychological and behavioral characteristics.
Nevertheless,
this is not to suggest that one should ignore the role of heredity as reflected
in heritability estimates altogether and focus on optimizing the environmental
factors for every child. Heredity, as has been examined, undoubtedly
contributes to the development of various human traits. Also, researchers
exploring environmental influences have found that contrary to what most
theorists expected, environmental factors that are shared by reared-together
twins do not appear to be relevant in explaining the development of particular
traits. It is therefore unlikely that exposing every child to a "one size
fits all" environment designed to foster a particular trait, would benefit
everyone equally. Some may react favorably to such an environment, while others
may not react to it at all; there may be yet others who react negatively to the
same environment. The notion of "range of reaction" helps us
conceptualize the complex relationship between heredity and environment; people
with varying genetically influenced predispositions respond differently to
environments. As suggested by Douglas Wahlsten in a 1994 article in Canadian
Psychology, an identical environment can elicit different reactions in
different individuals, due to variations in their genetic predispositions. In a
hypothetical scenario, Wahlsten suggested that increasing intellectual
stimulation should help increase cognitive performances of some children.
Moderate, rather than high, levels of intellectual stimulation may, however,
induce optimal cognitive performances in others. By contrast, the same moderate
levels of stimulation may actually cause some children to display cognitive
performances that are even worse than how they performed in a minimally
stimulating environment. In addition, the "optimal" or
"minimal" performance levels may be different for various
individuals, depending on their genetic makeup and other factors in their
lives. This example illustrates the individual differences in ranges of
reaction; there is no "recipe" for creating environments that
facilitate the development of particular characteristics in everyone. Heredity
via environment, rather than heredity versus environment, therefore, may better
characterize this perspective.
These
views are consistent with the 1990s' backlash against the view that was
prevalent in the mid- to late twentieth century among many clinical psychologists,
social workers, and educators, who focused solely on environmental factors
while discounting the contributions of hereditary factors. Among the theories
they advocated were that gay males decidedly come from families with
domineering mothers and no prominent masculine figures, that poor academic
performances result from lack of intellectual stimulation in early childhood,
and that autism stems from poor parenting practices. Not surprisingly,
empirical data do not support these theories. Still, people often continue to
believe, to some extent, that proper environments can prevent and
"cure" these non-normative characteristics, not realizing that
heredity may play significant roles in the development of these traits.
Some
scholars believe that this "radical environ-mentalist" view found its
popularity in the 1950s as a reaction to racist Nazi thinking, which held that
some groups of individuals are genetically inferior to others and that the
undesirable traits they are perceived to possess cannot be prevented or
modified. These assumptions are harmful, as they limit the opportunities for
advancement of some people, strictly because of their membership in a
stigmatized group. It is nevertheless important to reiterate that individual
differences, as opposed to group differences, in genetic predispositions are
evident in the development of most emotional, behavioral, and cognitive traits.
With this in mind, it is also important to realize that focusing on optimizing
environmental influences while ignoring hereditary influences may lead to the
neglect of the developmental needs of some individuals, and it may be just as
harmful in some cases as focusing exclusively on hereditary influences.
HUMAN
GROWTH AND DEVELOPMENT
MEANING
The meaning of words ‘growth and development’
is understood interchangeably. Terminology wise ‘Growth’ means increase and
enlargement of the body or some parts of the body e.g. body has become heavier
and larger etc. Thus growth is a change in the organism which can be observed
and measured in quantitative term. `Development’ means more advancement,
greater unfolding and growing forward to greater maturity. It refers to changes
over time which is not subject to quantitative measurement but express
themselves through certain behaviour pattern.
DEFINITION
- Development means a progressive series of
changes that occur in an orderly predictable pattern as a result of
maturation and experience - E.B.
Hurlock.
- Development means whole sequence of life from
conception to death - Pearsy
London
- Development is more important than ordinary
change. Development can be observed and to certain extent it can be
measured and evaluated. Its evaluation and measurement can be done in
three ways – Anatomic, Physiological and Behavioral. Behaviour signs
provide elaborate indices of the individual’s power and his level of
development – Gessel.
To sum up the above meaning ‘development’
is comprehensive in terms whereas `growth’ is one of the aspects of development
which refers to increase caused by becoming larger and heavier in size and
weight. ‘Development’ of the child can be defined as the emergence and
expansion of his capacities to provide greater facility in functioning. This
development is achieved through the process of growth, maturation and learning
which has two aspects of change; those of quantity and quality.
GROWTH
The term ‘Growth’ is
used in the physical sense. It generally refers to increase in size, length,
height and w eight. Changes in quantitative aspects, which could be objectively
observed and measured, come into the domain of growth. Growth is one of the components of
developmental process. Ina sense development in its quantitative aspect is
termed as growth.
Characteristics
of Growth:
1. Hereditary
factor is the cause.
2. Physical
factors play a dominant role.
2. Expansion
is height and weight is its apparent result.
3. It
is quantitative, additive and augmentative.
4. Growth
stops at a particular point of life.
5. Growth
need not necessarily cause development in all the cases.
6. Rate of growth is distinct and unique.
7. Individual
difference in growth is apparent and obvious.
DEVELOPMENT
Development is a qualitative and
integral change occurring at physical and mental levels improving the
efficiency or functional ability.
Characteristics
of Development:
1. Development
is a result of experience and maturation.
2. It
is a continuous process.
3.
It does not halt at puberty.
4. It
increases the intellectual, moral, social qualities.
5. It
is predictable and environment plays a dominant role.
6. Learning
is presupposition for development.
7. It
is difficult to measure in quantitative measures.
8. Mental
development could occur even without physical growth.
DIFFERENCES BETWEEN GROWTH AND DEVELOPMENT:
Sl. No.
|
Growth
|
Development
|
1.
|
Growth is quantitative and it shows an increase in size, weight and
height.
|
Development is both quantitative and qualitative.
|
2.
|
It is pertaining to physical and biological aspects.
|
It is pertaining to increasing the functional ability of an
individual.
|
3.
|
It is directional.
|
It is sequential and
progressive.
|
4.
|
It stops after the peak maturational level
|
It is a lifelong process.
|
5.
|
It could be objectively measured and exactly measured.
|
It cannot be measured easily. Yet keen and continuous observation
could reveal developmental levels.
|
6.
|
Motor and physical domain plays a dominant role.
|
Cognitive and affective domains play a vital role.
|
7.
|
It is not affected by learning.
|
Learning and experience has a lot of
impact on development.
|
8.
|
It is automatic in the sense that it does not demand much effort.
|
Development requires constant, continuous and guided efforts.
|
9.
|
Growth need not necessarily lead to development. It may or may not.
|
Development is integrative and includes mental, emotional, moral
aspects.
|
10.
|
Individual differences exist among children and it could be treated by
Physicians and therapeutic techniques.
|
Individual differences do exist in the learning skills and can not be
improved by clinical methods.
|
Conclusion:
Though there exists
many differences between growth and development, they are interrelated,
intertwined, complementary and not contradictory.
PRINCIPLES OF HUMAN GROWTH AND
DEVELOPMENT
INTRODUCTION
There is a set of principles that
characterizes the pattern and process of growth and development. These
principles or characteristics describe typical development as a predictable and
orderly process; that is, we can predict how most children will develop and
that they will develop at the same rate and at about the same time as other
children. Although there are individual differences in children's
personalities, activity levels, and timing of developmental milestones, such as
ages and stages, the principles and characteristics of development are universal
patterns.
PRINCIPLES
- Development proceeds from the head downward: This is called the cephalocaudal principle.
This principle describes the direction of growth and development.
According to this principle, the child gains control of the head first, then
the arms, and then the legs. Infants develop control of the head and face
movements within the first two months after birth. In the next few months,
they are able to lift themselves up by using their arms. By 6 to 12 months
of age, infants start to gain leg control and may be able to crawl, stand,
or walk. Coordination of arms always precedes coordination of legs.
- Development proceeds from the center of the
body outward: This is the principle of proximodistal
development that also describes the direction of development. This means
that the spinal cord develops before outer parts of the body. The child's
arms develop before the hands and the hands and feet develop before the
fingers and toes. Finger and toe muscles (used in fine motor dexterity)
are the last to develop in physical development.
- Development depends on maturation and learning: Maturation refers to the sequential characteristic of biological
growth and development. The biological changes occur in sequential order
and give children new abilities. Changes in the brain and nervous system
account largely for maturation. These changes in the brain and nervous
system help children to improve in thinking (cognitive) and motor
(physical) skills. Also, children must mature to a certain point before they
can progress to new skills (Readiness). For example, a four-month-old
cannot use language because the infant's brain has not matured enough to
allow the child to talk. By two years old, the brain has developed further
and with help from others, the child will have the capacity to say and
understand words. Also, a child can't write or draw until he has developed
the motor control to hold a pencil or crayon. Maturational patterns are
innate, that is, genetically programmed. The child's environment and the
learning that occurs as a result of the child's experiences largely
determine whether the child will reach optimal development. A stimulating
environment and varied experiences allow a child to develop to his or her
potential.
- Development proceeds from the simple
(concrete) to the more complex: Children use their cognitive and language skills to reason and
solve problems. For example, learning relationships between things (how
things are similar), or classification, is an important ability in
cognitive development. The cognitive process of learning how an apple and
orange are alike begins with the most simplistic or concrete thought of
describing the two. Seeing no relationship, a preschool child will
describe the objects according to some property of the object, such as
color. Such a response would be, "An apple is red (or green) and an
orange is orange." The first level of thinking about how objects are
alike is to give a description or functional relationship between the two
objects. "An apple and orange are round" and "An apple and
orange are alike because you eat them" are typical responses of
three, four and five year olds. As children develop further in cognitive
skills, they are able to understand a higher and more complex relationship
between objects and things; that is, that an apple and orange exist in a
class called fruit. The child cognitively is then capable of
classification.
- Growth and development is a continuous process: As a child develops, he or she adds to the skills already acquired
and the new skills become the basis for further achievement and mastery of
skills. Most children follow a similar pattern. Also, one stage of
development lays the foundation for the next stage of development. For
example, in motor development, there is a predictable sequence of
developments that occur before walking. The infant lifts and turns the
head before he or she can turn over. Infants can move their limbs (arms
and legs) before grasping an object. Mastery of climbing stairs involves
increasing skills from holding on to walking alone. By the age of four,
most children can walk up and down stairs with alternating feet. As in
maturation, in order for children to write or draw, they must have
developed the manual (hand) control to hold a pencil and crayon.
- Growth and development proceed from the
general to specific: In motor
development, the infant will be able to grasp an object with the whole
hand before using only the thumb and forefinger. The infant's first motor
movements are very generalized, undirected, and reflexive, waving arms or
kicking before being able to reach or creep toward an object. Growth
occurs from large muscle movements to more refined (smaller) muscle
movements.
- There are individual rates of growth and
development: Each child is different and the rates at
which individual children grow is different. Although the patterns and
sequences for growth and development are usually the same for all
children, the rates at which individual children reach developmental
stages will be different. Understanding this fact of individual
differences in rates of development should cause us to be careful about
using and relying on age and stage characteristics to describe or label
children. There is a range of ages for any developmental task to take
place. This dismisses the notion of the "average child". Some
children will walk at ten months while others walk a few months older at
eighteen months of age. Some children are more active while others are
more passive. This does not mean that the passive child will be less
intelligent as an adult. There is no validity to comparing one child's
progress with or against another child. Rates of development also are not
uniform within an individual child. For example, a child's intellectual
development may progress faster than his emotional or social development.
CONCLUSION
An understanding of the principles of
development helps us to plan appropriate activities and stimulating and
enriching experiences for children, and provides a basis for understanding how
to encourage and support young children's learning.
CHARACTERISTICS, DIMENSIONS OF
DEVELOPMENT
- Physical,
- Cognitive,
- Emotional,
- Social and
- Moral.
PHASES OF HUMAN DEVELOPMENT
The entire life span
of the individual is divided into various stages. We know that development is a
continuous process. All individuals have to pass through these following stages
of development.
PERIOD OR STAGES OF DEVELOPMENT
|
APPROXIMATE AGE
|
||
PRENATAL PERIOD
(From Conception to Birth)
|
1.
|
Germinal Period
|
First two weeks from
fertilization
|
2.
|
Embryonic Period
|
From Third to Eighth
weeks
|
|
3.
|
Fetal Period
|
From Ninth week to
birth
|
|
POSTNATAL PERIOD
(From Birth to Death)
|
1.
|
Infancy
|
From birth to 2 years
|
2.
|
Childhood
|
From 3 years to12 years or up to the onset of puberty
|
|
a. Early Childhood
|
From 3 years to 6 years
|
||
b. Later Childhood
|
From 7 years to 12 years or up to the onset of
puberty
|
||
3.
|
Adolescence
|
From 13 years to 19 years or from onset of
puberty till attainment of maturity
|
|
a. Early Adolescence
|
From 13 to 16 years
|
||
b. Later Adolescence
|
From 17 to 19 years
|
||
4.
|
Adulthood
|
From 20 years to 60 years
|
|
a. Early Adulthood
|
From 20 to 40 years
|
||
b. Later Adulthood
|
From 41 to 60 years
|
||
5
|
Old Age
|
From 61 years to death
|
INFANCY
INTRODUCTION:
- MEDICAL
CIRCLES: An infant is referred to a person who is incapable of speech and
is thus helpless.
- LEGALLY: A
person is regarded an infant until he reaches the age of eighteen. A
minor, according to the law, is thus an infant.
- PSYCHOLOGICAL
RESEARCH CENTRES: Refer to the first 2 years of life as infancy.
- ELIZABETH B.
HURLOCK has spanned INFANCY from birth till 2 weeks and attributed the
term BABYHOOD from the period extending from the end of the second week
after birth until the end of the second year of life.
- GENERALLY:
Infancy refers to the period of development that begins at birth and ends
about 18 months to 2 years of age with early language use.
MODERN UNDERSTANDING OF INFANCY:
Although it comprises only 2 percent of the life
span, infancy is one of the most remarkable and busiest times of development.
The new born baby or neonate, (refers to infants from birth through the first
month of life) enters the world with surprisingly
sophisticated perceptual and motor abilities, a set of skills for interacting
with people, and a capacity to learn
that is put to use immediately after birth. By the end of infancy, the small child is a sociable, self-assertive, purposeful being who walks on her own,
has developed refined manual skills, and is prepared to acquire the most unique
of human abilities-language.
Our view of the infant has changed drastically over
this century. At one time, the new born baby was considered to be a passive, incompetent being whose world
was, in the words of turn-of-the-century psychologist William James, “A blooming, buzzing confusion”. Recently developed
methods and equipments permitting researchers to test the young baby’s
capacities have shown this image to be wrong. It is now well accepted that the
infants, from the outset, are skilled and capable beings who display the
beginnings of many complex abilities.
GENERAL NOTIONS OF INFANCY:
- ‘Age of Dependence’: The infant is totally dependent on the
caretaker to fulfill all its needs.
- ‘Age of Rapid Growth and Change’: Growth is both physical and psychological.
The bodily movements are more coordinated and the infant is able to
recognize and identify people and objects,
- ‘The Foundation Age’: At this period of time, the foundations of
many behaviour, patterns, attitude to others and self and emotional
expressions are being established.
- ‘A Shy Age’: The infant’s world is limited to the family and to significant
people. It shies away from strangers and unfamiliar surroundings.
- ‘A Hazardous Age’: The child is prone to physical illnesses and
accidents. The high infant mortality rate is evidence of this. Also
psychological damage.
- ‘An Appealing Age’: Adults as well as older children find the
infant appealing because of its helplessness, dependency and easy to
manage feeling that one gets.
NEWBORN REFLEXES:
A reflex is an inborn, automatic response to a
particular form of stimulation. Reflexes are the neonate’s most obvious
organized patterns of behaviour. Like breathing and swallowing, some newborns
reflexes have survival value. Infants come into the world with dozens of them.
They are as follows:
- Rooting (Head
turns toward source of stimulation)
- Sucking
(Infant sucks finger rhythmically)
- Swimming (Baby
paddles and kicks in swimming motion)
- Eye blink
(Infant closes eye lids quickly)
- Withdrawal
(Foot withdraws, with flexion of knee and hip)
- Babinski
(Toes fan out and curls as foot twists in)
- Moro (Infant
makes an ‘embracing’ motion)
- Palmar grasp
(Infant grasps adult finger)
- Tonic neck
(Infant assumes a ‘fencing position’)
- Body righting
(Rest of body turns in same direction)
- Stepping
(Infant lifts one foot after another in stepping response)
PHYSICAL DEVELOPMENT:
- At birth, the
average infant weighs 7.5 pounds and measures 19.5 inches in length.
- During the 1st
6 months, growth continues at a rapid rate and then begins to slow down in
the second year.
- In the 1st
year there is increase in weight to approximately 21 pounds and in the
second year the height increases between 28 and 30 inches.
- Head growth
slows down while trunk and limb growth increases.
- The number of
bones increases and ossification begins.
- Out of the 20
temporary teeth, about 16 appear by the age of 2 years.
- Growth in the
nervous system consists primarily of the development of immature cells
present at birth, rather than the formation of new cells.
- Eye muscles
are well coordinated. Can distinguish colours. Skin stimuli are highly
responsive because of the thin texture of the skin.
Pattern of Motor Control:
i) Head region:- (a) Eye control
(b) Starts smiling (c) Can hold
the head erect.
ii) Trunk region:- (a) Can roll over completely by 6 months. (b) Is able
to sit by 7 months.
iii) Arms:- Can reach objects and can pick up small objects by 1 year.
iv) Legs:- Can walk without support at 14 months.
Pre-speech forms of Communication:
(1) Crying (2) Babbling (3) Gesturing with words (4) Emotional
expressions
EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT:
Common Emotional Patterns:
- Anger: When interfered with its movements, unable to do a task, for not
being understood, the child reacts by screaming, kicking, waving the arms,
jumps up and down, et al.
- Fear: Any stimulus which occurs suddenly or unexpectedly or which is
different from what the infant is accustomed to gives rise to fear.
- Curiosity: Anything new or unusual acts as a stimulus to curiosity. They
express curiosity mainly through their facial expressions.
- Joy: is stimulated by physical well-being and friendly responses of
others. They are expressed through smiling, laughing, baby coos, gurgles
or even shouts with glee, and all bodily movements are intensified.
- Affection: Family members, toys and family pets stimulate the infant’s
affection which it expresses by hugging, patting and kissing.
SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT:
Infancy is centered round home and therefore it is
here that the foundations for later social behaviour and attitudes are formed.
A happy environment at home enhances the social adjustability while infants who
cried excessively tend to become aggressive.
Socialising Behaviour of the Infant:
- At birth it
makes no difference who takes care of its physical needs.
- By 2-3 months
it distinguishes people from inanimate objects.
- By 4-5 months
loves to be picked up and reacts differently to friendly and angry voices.
- 6-7 months is
the beginning of the ‘shy age’. It smiles at ‘friends’ and shows fear in
the presence of ‘strangers’. It is attracted to other babies.
- 8-9 months it
attempts to imitate the speech, gestures and simple acts of others.
- 20-24 months
it co-operates in routine activities like being dressed, fed, and bathed.
Is interested in playing with other babies and uses play materials to
establish social relationship with them.
MENTAL DEVELOPMENT:
According to Piaget this is the ‘Sensorimotor Stage’ in conceptual
development. Its earliest perceptions come through sensory exploration. It
understands by:
- Looking,
listening, smelling and tasting anything that comes its way.
- Tries to
discover meaning through simpler forms by asking the questions ‘who’,
‘what’, ‘why’.
- It associates
ideas with objects and situations resulting in the development of concepts.
Develops concepts of space, weight, time, self, social, beauty and
comic.
MORAL DEVELOPMENT:
The infant is non-moral
as his behaviour is not guided by moral standards. The rightness or wrongness of its actions is judged by the ‘Pleasure-Pain Principle’ it has on him.
It perceives the act as good or bad if it brings him pleasure or pain
respectively. It has no sense
of guilt as it has no concept of
personal property. Therefore, it does not feel bad in taking the things of
others. The infant is in the 1st stage of moral development which
Piaget terms as ‘Morality by Constraint’.
CONCLUSION:
It is in infancy that the reservoir of security is formed, from which love, affection and compassion may be drawn all the rest of life. It is also in infancy
that the basic orientation towards
learning is established. The nature and condition of the child’s learning
may alter as it grows older, but the first learning’s appear to be fundamental
and at least to a degree determine the later attitudes. The relevance of basic
trust to later living and learning implies that adequate loving care in infancy may be a potent factor in the ultimate realization of full human
potential.
CHILDHOOD
INTRODUCTION
Education
aims at the all round harmonious development of an individual. The development
of a nation depends upon the development of its children and there is no doubt
that the childhood is the foundation upon which the development of an
individual depends.
Childhood, the period between three and twelve years of age, is an
extremely impressionable age, where all the little things that the child learns
in the starting years will be carried over with him into adulthood. This period
is divided into the period of early
childhood and the period of late childhood.
A. Early Childhood:
This period of early childhood ranges from 3-6
years. From the point of view, childhood is a period of consolidation of
infancy and babyhood. Generally parents consider the ‘Early Childhood’
a problem age or troublesome age. Educators
consider it pre-school age or toy age. While some psychologists’
name it pre-gang age, some call it exploratory or questioning
age and most of them say it imitative age or creative
age.
Characteristics:
- Physical
development continues at a slower rate because of which his hunger may
decrease temporarily.
- Muscles are
more evenly distributed and muscular coordination is increased. The
control of elimination moves towards perfection in these years.
- The bones
develop in size and shape.
- The nervous
system becomes more mature.
- There is
rapid increase in vocabulary and the child can express his thoughts and
emotions in simple sentences. He can relate stories.
- The power of
imitation is very active.
- The child
seeks to gain control over his environment. His instincts such as
curiosity, self-display, and acquisition become active.
- The child
learns to make social adjustments. He develops emotional attachment to his
parents and other family members, and learns primary social manners.
- The child has
imaginary friends. There is an increase in the fear of imaginary
creatures, the dark, and the animals.
- The child in
this stage learns `I’, `me’, and `you’.
B. Late Childhood:
The period of 6 -12 years is considered as late
childhood period. This age is known by the educators as elementary school age
and psychologists call it gang age and play age.
Characteristics:
- Increase
in the size of the body is the main characteristic of this stage. Physical
growth becomes slow and steady. There is remarkable growth in height,
weight and improvement in motor skills.
- The
major development is building up friends with peer team spirit. Social
development takes place through the social experiences.
- Maturity
occurs rapidly. Girls mature faster than boys.
- At this
age children move from egocentric to socialized speech and thoughts.
- The
self-concept emerges in children.
- The
children show interests in comics, movies, and television.
- Moral
development also takes place. The child’s conscience begins to develop in
identification with parents.
- Affection,
sympathy, and cooperation among the group members emerge during this
period.
- Emotional
stability in the conduct and behaviour of the child takes place. He learns
to have control over his emotional expressions.
- Control
over the mind is also an important aspect of this stage. Most children
enjoy the feeling of having learned new and difficult things.
AREAS OF DEVELOPMENT
1. Physical development:
Physical development can be defined as the series
of anatomic and physiologic changes taking place between the beginning of
pre-natal life and senility.
1.
The child begins
to assume the body proportions for an adult.
2.
Height: Generally
after two and half years the child gains 2-2.5 inches every year up to 12
years. Approximately the height of male
child becomes 45 inches and female 43 inches.
3.
Weight: There is
steady increase in weight, average growth per year 5 to 6 pounds.
4.
Legs growth is
rapid and represent about half of ones total height
5.
The head is slow
in growth but the trunk grows a little faster.
6.
Towards puberty
girls are a bit lighter and shorter.
7.
At this age muscle
develops at rapid speed.
2. Motor development:
Motor development means development of strength, speed
and accuracy in the use of muscular parts of the body such as arms, eyes, legs
and neck muscles. Motor abilities involve bodily movements of various organs
and coordinated functions of nerves and muscles.
- Motor
development adopts two directions - from head to foot and from the centre
of the body to outward.
- Motor
development occurs in four major areas - the head region, the trunk, the
arms and hands, and legs and feet.
- Childhood is
called the ideal age of learning motor skills. There are
three reasons.
- Young
children enjoy repetition and are willing to repeat an activity until
they have acquired the ability to do it well.
- Young
children are adventuresome.
- Young
children learn easily and quickly because their bodies are still pliable.
- Spontaneous
catching a ball, throwing a ball begins at 2-5 years onwards.
- Painting,
writing.
- Standing,
walking, running, climbing, skipping, hopping jumping and kicking the ball
etc.
3. Emotional development:
The word ‘emotion’ is derived from the Latin word ‘Emovere’ which means
to stir up, to agitate or to excite. According to R.S. Woods Worth (1945) “Emotion is a moved
or stirred up state of an organism. It is a stirred up state of feeling that is
the way it appears to the individual himself. It is a disturbed muscular and
glandular activity- that is the way it appears to an external observer.” Every
child is born with the potential expression. Emotion is a label for vast range
of psychosomatic states which involve feeling, perception, or awareness of an
event or circumstances. The word emotion covers conditions of both positive and
negative character. The condition in which an individual is eager, zestful,
jubilant and moved is referred to as positive condition, while on the other
hand the condition in which an individual feels disturbed, distressed and moved
away is referred as negative condition.
Common
childhood emotions:
1. Fear:
As the children pass from the pre-school to the
elementary school they are faced with certain fears.
v Children of six years show fear for the
supernatural beings such as ghosts and witches.
v By seven years children show fear for quarrels,
wars and spies etc.
v Fear of failure and not being liked by people is
common among the children of 8 years. Ten to 12 year olds are fearful of things
as being in the dark, animals.
2. Anger and rivalry against siblings
3. Jealousy
4. Joy and pleasure
5. Love and affection
4. Mental or intellectual development:
Mental development implies the progressive changes
in the mental process which goes on from birth to death. Mental development
includes various aspects such as development of concepts, perception, language,
memory, reasoning, thinking, intelligence and imagination. Mental growth continues in childhood also.
The child seeks to satisfy his voracious curiosity by asking an infinite
variety of questions to his parents. That is why this age is called a
questioning age or an exploratory age.
- At this stage
reflex actions and instincts develop
- Child’s
interests grow in number and extents.
- Child
develops interests in reading short stories.
- Child spends
much time in reading history and visiting new places.
- At this time
power of memory increases.
- At 6 years
child displays ability to distinguish between right and left, to count up
to 13 or 14 objects, to solve problems.
- At 7 years
child develops to distinguish between two objects.
- At 8 years
child is able to repeat sentences containing 16 to 17 words.
- At 9 years
child becomes conscious of day, time, date, recognizes coins of various
denominations. Thus new concepts are picked up.
- At 10 years
child points out mistakes in short sentences and repeats 60-70 words in
three minutes.
- At 11 years
child can recognize similarity and differences, make comparisons,
distinguish between male and female, as well as birds and animals.
- At 12 years
child points out the cause of a thing and provides his own explanation.
5. Language development:
A major feature that distinguishes the human
being from animal is their ability to use vocal speech as a means of
communication. Towards the end of babyhood 2-3 years child begins to use words
and forming sentences. At 4-5 years child improves to form sentences
considerably and is able to use grammar. After 5 he achieves comprehension
of meanings of the words, builds up new
vocabulary e.g. good, bad, this, that and learns to have correct pronunciation,
identifies sound of the certain difficult letters e.g. Z, W, Sh, Stir etc.
Language development according to Scissore is as follows:
Age in years
|
Words
|
4
|
5,600
|
5
|
9,600
|
6
|
14,700
|
7
|
21,200
|
8
|
26,300
|
10
|
34,300
|
Hyder brothers concluded from their studies that
a. linguistic development is
faster in the case of girls than that of boys
b. girls construct longer
sentences than boys
c. girls are more skilled in expressing their
thoughts coherently.
The child learns three forms of language namely
spoken, written and gesture. The individual’s linguistic development is
profoundly influenced by the community, home, school, and the family’s social
and economic status. Besides these important factors affecting are his
intelligence, health, maturation, sex and personality.
6. Social development:
1.
Social development
is the process of learning to conform to group standards - Freeman and Showel.
2.
Social development
means acquisition of the ability to behave in accordance with social
expectations - Crow and Crow.
3.
Social development
means the attaining of maturity in social setting- E.B. Hurlock.
A social child behaves in a socially approved
manner, plays the role which society prescribes for him and has favourable
attitude towards people and social activities.
·
Between 2-6 years
children learn to make social contacts and get along with people outside home,
especially with children of their own.
·
In childhood,
children have strong desires to be with children and to be accepted by them.
·
Peer groups have
greater impact on some children than on others.
·
Children show
kindness to others and defend the rights of younger children
·
Friendship at late
childhood can easily be made and broken.
·
Maintain
individual friendships.
·
Girls have groups.
·
8-10 years (gang
age) often reject adult’s standard.
Characteristics of social development
- Early
social response
- Response
to other children
- Group
activities
- Social
perception
- Resistant
behavior
- Sympathy
- Fights and
quarrels
- Cooperation
- Competition
7. Moral development:
The term moral comes from the Latin word mores which means manners, custom,
and folkways. Moral behaviour means human behviour in conformity with moral
code of social group. It is carried out in a voluntary way. Moral behaviour is
accompanied by a feeling of a personal responsibility for one’s acts. In this
respect the young child’s intellectual development has not yet reached the
point where he can learn or apply abstract principles of right and wrong. The
child has to learn what is good and what is bad which is a slow process.
Piaget has divided
moral development into three parts of childhood in which the child learns such behaviour:
1.
During this period
(4-5) he begins to imitate the actions of parents and others who are in close
touch with him.
2.
At 5-8 years he
learns to respect parents, teachers, and other adults. Morality of obedience
and duty is inculcated happily and spontaneously.
3.
At this age (9-13)
there is the morality of cooperation through free, reciprocal relationship with
his peers. Child develops a morality of mutual respect and cooperation. This
stage is considered as the golden year of life because experiences of the child of this period remain
life long.
Moral development of child implies inculcation in
the child a number of qualities for which curriculum provides ample
opportunities. E.g. Honesty in words and deeds, truthfulness, self respect and
desire to respect others, righteousness, self control, duty consciousness and
compassion. In spite of hazards the child develops conscience. Here the role of
discipline is very essential in the moral development of the child.
CONCLUSION
Childhood is the time when the individual’s basic
outlooks, values and ideals are to a great extent shaped. The experiences a
child has at school, home and larger community during these formative years
well determine, for example, whether he is to be a fearful child or a one
possessed with confidence in himself or whether he will be tolerant or
intolerant towards others.
‘No period during the life cycle is more important
than childhood from an educational point of view.’
ADOLESCENCE -------------------------------
DEVELOPMENTAL TASKS &
EDUCATION
Definition:
Each cultural
group recognizes that certain skills and behaviour patters can be mastered at
certain age. These skills are necessary for the individual’s adjustment in
social life. So, the cultural group expects its member to master certain skills
and acquire cetain approved patterns of behaviour at various ages during the
life span. These are known as the developmental
task.
Havighurst (1972)
defines a developmental task is ‘a task which arises at or about a certain
period in the life of the individual, successful achievement of which leads to
happiness and to success with later tasks, while failure leads to unhappiness,
social disapproval, and difficulty with later tasks. Three forces work together
in mastering developmental task. They are
1. Physical
maturation,
2. Cultural
pressures of society and
3. Personal values
& aspirations of the individual.
For
e.g. motor skills like walking develop largely as a result of physical
maturation. Learning to read develops from the cultural pressure of the
society. Choosing and preparing for a career grows out of the personal values
and aspirations of the individual.
According to our biopsychosocial model, the first
source corresponds to the "bio" part of the model, the second to the
"psycho," and the third to the "social" aspect. Havighurst has identified six major age
periods:
1.
Infancy and Early
Childhood (0-5 years),
2.
Later Childhood
(6-12 years),
3.
Adolescence (13-18
years),
4.
Early Adulthood
(19-29 years),
5.
Middle Adulthood
(30-60 years), and
6.
Later Maturity
(61+).
Purposes:
Developmental task serve three very useful
purposes.
- Developmental task is the guidance that
enables a person to know what society expects from them at a given age.
- Individuals are motivated to do what the
social group expects them to do at (certain ages) different periods of the
life span.
- It indicates what lies ahead and what will be
expected to do when they reach the next stage of development.
Hazards:
There are three very common potential hazards
related to developmental tasks.
- The first one is the inappropriate
expectations; either individual themselves or the social group may expect
the development of behaviour that is impossible at the time because of
physical or psychological limitations.
- The second one is the Bypassing of a stage of
development as results of failure of master the tasks for that stage of
development.
- The crises individuals experience when they
pass from one stage to another comprise the third one.
Factors
influencing mastery of developmental task:
- A retarded developmental level
- Lack of opportunity to learn developmental tasks or lack of
guidance in their mastery
- Lack of motivation
- Poor health
- Physical defects
- A low intellectual level
Implications:
- The concept of developmental task is very
helpful in specifying course’s content and its objectives.
- The pupil can know in advance, what the
society expects from his/her at that age.
- Parents and teachers can guide the children to
acquire skills to live in the society.
- The teachers and parents can prepare
appropriate climate or atmosphere to achieve a developmental task.
DEVELOPMENTAL
TASK OF INFANCY AND EARLY CHILDHOOD (From 0 to 5 years)
1) Learning to walk: Once the basic skills are mastered, he
learns during later years to run, jump, and skip.
2) Learning to take solid foods: The way the child is treated
during the weaning period, the schedule on which he is fed, and the age and
suddenness of weaning, all have profound effects upon his personality.
3) Learning to talk: Between these ages of twelve and eighteen
months, the great moment of speech arrives. The two theories agree to this
extent, namely (1) that the human infant develops a repertory of speech -
sounds without having to learn them, and (2) that the people around him teach
him to attach certain meanings to these sounds.
4) Learning to control the elimination of body waste: To learn
to urinate and defecate at socially acceptable times and places. Toilet
training is the first moral training that the child receives. The stamp of this
first moral training probably persists in the child's later character.
5) Learning sex differences and sexual modesty: The kinds
of sexual behavior he learns and the attitudes and feelings he develops about
sex in these early years probably have an abiding effect upon his sexuality
throughout his life.
6) Achieving physiological stability: It takes as many as five
years for the child's body to settle down to something like the physiological
stability of the child.
7) Forming simple concepts of social and physical reality: And,
when his nervous system is ready, he must have the experience and the teachers
to enable him to form a stock of concepts and learn the names for them. On this
basis his later mental development is built.
8) Learning to relate oneself emotionally to parents, siblings, and
other people: The way he achieves this task of relating himself
emotionally to other people will have a large part in determining whether he
will be friendly or cold, outgoing or introversive, in his social relations in
later life.
9) Learning to distinguish right and wrong and developing a conscience:
During the later years of early childhood he takes into himself the warning and
punishing voices of his parents, in ways that depend upon their peculiar
displays of affection and punishment toward him. Thus he develops the bases of
his conscience, upon which a later structure of values and moral character will
be built.
DEVELOPMENTAL TASK OF LATER CHILDHOOD (From
6 to 12 years)
1. Learning physical skills necessary for ordinary games: To learn
the physical skills that are necessary for the games and physical activities
that are highly valued in childhood--such skills as throwing and catching,
kicking, tumbling, swimming, and handling simple tools.
2. Building a wholesome attitudes toward oneself as a growing
organism: To develop habits of care of the body, of
cleanliness and safety consistent with a wholesome, realistic attitude which
includes a sense of physical normality and adequacy, the ability to enjoy using
the body, and a wholesome attitude toward sex. Sex education should be a matter
of agreement between school and parents, with the school doing what the parents
feel they cannot do so well. The facts about animal and human reproduction
should be taught before puberty.
3. Learning to get along with age-mates: To learn
the give-and-take of social life among peers. To learn to make friends and to
get along with enemies. To develop a "social personality."
4. Beginning to develop appropriate masculine or feminine social roles: To learn to
be a boy or a girl--to act the role that is expected and rewarded. The sex role
is taught so vigorously by so many agencies that the school probably has little
more than a remedial function, which is to assist boys and girls who are having
difficulty with the task.
5. Developing fundamental skills in reading, writing and calculating: To learn to
read, write, and calculate well enough to get along in society.
6. Developing concepts necessary for everyday living: The task is
to acquire a store of concepts sufficient for thinking effectively about
ordinary occupational, civic, and social matters.
7. Developing a conscience, a sense of morality and a scale of
values: To develop an inner moral control, respect for
moral rules, and the beginning of a rational scale of values. Morality or
respect for rules of behavior is imposed on the child first by the parents.
Later, according to Piaget, the child learns that rules are necessary and
useful to the conduct of any social enterprise, from games to government, and
thus learns a "morality of cooperation or agreement" which is a true
moral autonomy and necessary in a modern democratic society.
8. Achieving personal independence: To become
an autonomous person, able to make plans and to act in the present and
immediate future independently of one's parents and other adults. The young
child has become physically independent of his parents but remains emotionally
dependent on them.
9. Developing attitudes toward social groups and institutions: To develop
social attitudes those are basically democratic. Attitudes are learned mainly
in three ways; (1) by imitation of people with prestige in the eyes of the
learner; (2) by collection and combination of pleasant or unpleasant
experiences associated with a given object or situation; (3) by a single deeply
emotional experience--pleasant or unpleasant--associated with a given object or
situation.
DEVELOPMENTAL TASK OF ADOLESCENCE (From
13 to 18 years)
1. Achieving new and more mature relations with age-mates of both
sexes: To learn to look upon girls as women and boys as men; to become an
adult among adults; to learn to work with others for a common purpose,
disregarding personal feelings; to learn to lead without dominating.
2. Achieving a masculine or feminine social role: To accept
and to learn a socially approved adult masculine or feminine social role.
3. Accepting one's physique and using one’s body effectively: To become
proud, or at least tolerant, of one's body; to use and protect one's body
effectively and with personal satisfaction.
4. Achieving emotional independence from parents and other adults: To become
free from childish dependence on parents; to develop affection for parents
without dependence upon them.
5. Achieving assurance of economic independence: To feel
able to make a living, if necessary. This is primarily a task for boys, in our
society, but it is of increasing importance to girls.
6. Selecting and preparing for an occupation: To choose
an occupation for which one has the necessary ability; to prepare for this
occupation.
7. Preparing for marriage and family life: To develop
a positive attitude toward family life and having children; and (mainly for
girls) to get the knowledge necessary for home management and child rearing.
8. Developing intellectual skills and concepts necessary for civic
competence: To develop concepts of law, government, economics,
politics, geography, human nature, and social institutions which fit the modern
world; to develop language skills and reasoning ability necessary for dealing
effectively with the problems of a modern democracy. Individual differences in
mental development show themselves principally as differences in: (a) acquiring
language and meanings, (2) acquiring concepts, (3) interests and motivation.
9. Desiring, accepting and achieving socially responsible behavior: To
participate as a responsible adult in the life of the community, region, and
nation; to take account of the values of society in one's personal behavior.
10. Acquiring a set of values and an ethical system as a guide
behavior - developing an ideology: To form a set of values that are
possible of realization; to develop a conscious purpose of realizing these
values; to define man's place in the physical world and in relation to other
human beings; to keep one's world picture and one's values in harmony with each
other.
CONCLUSION
The developmental tasks concept has a long and rich
tradition. Its acceptance has been partly due to recognition of sensitive
periods in our lives and partly due to the practical nature of Havighurst's
tasks. Knowing that a youngster of a certain age is encountering one of the
tasks of that period (learning an appropriate sex role) helps adults to
understand a child's behavior and establish an environment that helps the child
to master the tasks. Another good example is that of acquiring personal
independence, an important task for the middle childhood period. Youngsters
test authority during this phase and, if teachers and parents realize that this
is a normal, even necessary phase of development, they react differently than
if they see it as a personal challenge.
No comments:
Post a Comment